Sheryl Dare asks the students in her Ideas in Western
Literature class to design and carrry out an independent project
which examines a cultural assumption. Here is the assignment sheet
which frames the project for the students, and a sample student
response.
The purpose of the project is twofold. It's an opportunity to 1 )
exercise your academic FREEDOM in connection with some investigation
of the TRUTH, by designing and implementing a collaborative project
in a way that 2) takes some aspect of the putative KNOWLEDGE you've
gained in this course and put it into ACTION.
Translated into plain language, Phase III asks that you identify,
define, gather evidence for, critically think and come to some
conclusion about a cultural assumption. The content and actual format
can be anything you want them to be. BUT, be reminded that with
freedom comes the burden of RESPONSIBILITY (implications and
consequences), so please do nothing dangerous, hurtful or
illegal.
Here are some of the concepts we've touched on in the course reading
that you may want to explore. The concepts, along with references to
the texts we've read, must be embedded in your final paper.
Freedom
Truth
Individual vs. Society (Plato, Cuckoo)
Post-modern bankruptcy of values (Kafka, Hollow Men, 2nd Coming, No Exit) Consciousness (Sisyphus, Hamlet)
Obedience
Ethics (Job, Ishrnael, Grand Inquisitor)
Knowledge = Action = Responsibility (Maslow, Hamlet)
Any or all elements of the CT process
You will be assessed according to these common descriptors of
Understanding:
1. Interpretation: How did you interpret the raw data you gathered in
order to arrive at your conclusions? Did you account for your own
biases in your interpretation?
2. Application: How have you applied the knowledge you've gained in
this class in some tangible way?
3. Perspective: Can you step back and see your problem/hypothesis in
some Bigger Picture? Can you see the problem from different points of
view? Consequences and implications leading from different courses of
action?
4. Self-Knowledge (meta-cognition): What did this project teach you
about yourselves? About the way/s you think?
5. Explanation: You (as a group) will need to be able to explain the
parameters of your project to the class orally, and to me in writing.
(5 pages typewritten, doublespaced, not including your raw data).
Sara A.
Sara C.
Tiara K.
Testing Our Tastes
Phase 111 Project
Purpose: To test the cultural assumption that more expensive,
more popular products are of a higher quality than cheaper, more
generic brands.
Hypothesis: Our personal opinion has more to do with what we
hear from the media and those around us than our actual individual
taste.
Materials: Coca Cola Classic Soda (~ six-pack for $2.59)
Springfield Cola (1 six-pack for $1.69) 24 taste testers
Method: Out of the 24 taste testers randomly selected to
participate in our experiment, half were subjected to Test#1 while
the remaining half were subjected to Test #2. Test #1 involved
preparing two small cups of cola, one containing the more expensive
and popular Coca Cola Classic Soda and the other containing the
cheaper, generic Springfield Cola. We did not inform the taste
testers of the contents in each cup and asked them to sample each
cola and tell us which they preferred. Test #2 involved preparing the
two cups as we did in Test #1. However, this time, we revealed to the
taste tester which cup contained Coca Cola Classic and which
contained Springfield Cola. Once again, we asked them which cola they
preferred. In both tests, we recorded the decision of each taste
tester and drew conclusions from our compiled data.
Data:
Test One
Springfield Cola 58% (1/12)
Coca-Cola 42% (5/12)
Test Two
Springfield Cola 25% (3/12)
Coca Cola 75% (9/12)
Conclusion:
For the most part, the results of our experiment support our initial
hypothesis that our opinions are influenced more by society than our
actual personal taste. As we predicted, when we did not inform the
taste testers of the brand of cola they were sampling, more taste
testers indicated a preference for the cheaper, more generic
Springfield Cola than when they were aware of which cola they were
drinking. This suggests that our personal tastes are largely
manipulated by outside societal influences such as the media and
those around us... that we are more a product of society than one of
our own making. If we know that a product is more expensive and more
"popular" than another product, we often assume that it is of higher
quality and therefore "better". Almost every day, we encounter some
type of Coca Cola Classic promotion: commercials... magazine
advertisements, a classmate quenching their thirst with the
all-American beverage of choice. Even the Punahou School cafeteria
serves all soft drinks in a bright red cup emblazoned with the
oh-so-familiar white, cursive letters of the Coca Cola Classic logo,
and the majority of soda machines we encounter showcase Coca Cola
Classic above the hundreds of other sodas on the market. It is
therefore not surprising that a taste tester would "prefer" Coca Cola
Classic over a generic brand of cola they've never even heard of.
That is, of course, only if they are aware of which cola is which.
However, when taste testers are kept in the dark, and, presumably,
left to make a decision entirely based on personal taste rather than
one influenced by cultural assumptions and stereotypes, our results
show that Coca Cola Classic, though it is more expensive, more
advertised and more popular than Springfield Cola, is not necessarily
the preferred cola.
The results of our experiment reveal several assumptions that factor
into our everyday choices. For example, the fact that when people
were not informed of which cola they were drinking more people chose
Springfield Cola than when they were informed of the difference
implies that many of us assume that higher priced, more popular
products are superior to lesser-known, cheaper products. We
automatically correlate price and popularity with quality, often
forgetting to even consider our actual personal preference. We assume
that what we see and hear in advertisements and from those around us
is the truth, often adapting our own "truth" to the popular "truth."
We ask ourselves "What do most people like?" and "What should I
like?" rather than "What do I like?" We define our "selves" by
societal standards rather than discovering our own true identity. Our
results show that people not only "prefer" Coca Cola Classic to the
more generic Springfield Cola, but that they actually pay nearly
twice as much for this brand name cola for reasons, apparently, often
having nothing to do with their personal taste. Furthermore, our
decisions are also often affected by the societal assumption that
there is some correlation between what we buy and who we are. Many
people assume that those who buy name brand products (which are
usually high priced) are "high class" while they pity or look down
upon those that buy cheaper, more generic brands as "poor," "cheap"
or "low class."
Several connections can be drawn between the results of our
experiment and the texts we have been reading and discussing
throughout the semester. In his novel Ishmael Daniel Quinn
depicts our civilization as one influenced by an invisible force he
calls "Mother Culture" which encroaches into every corner of our
lives, dictating what we believe, how we act and who we are.
Similarly, in Plato's "Allegory of the Cave," he asserts that society
is a cave in which we are restrained by chains. The media, popular
opinion and other external factors that influence our opinions can be
considered "Mother Culture" in disguise: the chains that bind, the
veils that blind. The mighty powers of Mother Culture were at work
while our informed taste testers selected their cola of choice; yet
our uninformed taste testers, released from their chains in the form
of assumptions and stereotypes they might attach to each cola, were
far less vulnerable to her evil clutches. Quinn describes our
civilization as being ensnared behind invisible bars constructed by
"Mother Culture." Thus, as our informed taste testers sampled their
cola, they were trapped behind and restrained by cultural assumptions
that prevented them from being true to themselves. Furthermore, the
fact that we are often unaware of these assumptions and their
restrictive influences renders us less likely to escape them. As
Quinn posits, "If you can't discover what's keeping you in, the will
to get out soon becomes confused and ineffectual" (25).
Similar to "Mother Culture" which influences our choices, "The
Combine," which symbolizes society in Ken Kesey's novel One Flew
Over the Cuckoo's Nest, also controls the way we behave and act.
The Combine encompasses the "norms" of society, rejecting any type of
behavior that deviates frorn what is deemed normal or conventional by
popular belief. This connects to the results we observed in our
experiment, which showed that people are more inclined to "choose"
the more popular, accepted Coca Cola Classic instead of the generic,
unconventional Springfield cola when they are aware of which brand
they are drinking. As mentioned before, assumptions about these two
products influence people's choices. By purchasing a generic brand
that is considered unconventional by society, a person is more
vulnerable to negative stereotypes, since his or her choice goes
against the accepted "norm." Nurse Ratched, who is an enforcer of the
Combine, advocates conforming to the rules of society. She tells the
patients, "A good many of you are in here because you could not
adjust to the rules of society in the Outside World, because you
refused to face up to them, because you tried to circumvent them and
avoid them" (171). By articulating the consequences of defying or
circumventing societal rules, Nurse Ratched suggests that it is in a
person's best interest to choose the "conventional" path. Thus, this
explains why people subjected to Test #2 were more inclined to choose
Coca Cola Classic. By conforming to popular taste, which is a product
of the media and other external societal forces, people are more
likely to be accepted by society, which often criminalizes
difference. People's desire to be accepted by others often affects
the way they behave and the choices they make, as supported by the
results of our experiment.
The results of our experiment can also be interpreted in light of
Susan Blackmore's portrayal of the "self' in her article about memes
and genes. In her article, she asserts that, "the idea of 'self is an
illusion." She defines "memes" as "ideas, skills, habits, stories,
songs or inventions that are passed from person to person by
imitation." Blackmore argues that our "selves" are simply an
accumulation of memes absorbed from the world around us. Our opinions
and beliefs are a reflection of the memes we successfully absorb...
we are the end product of a sort of 'survival of the fittest." In the
end, "the choices [we] make are not made by an inner self who
has free will, but are just the consequences of the replicators
playing out their competition in a particular environment. In the
process they create the illusion of a self who is in control." With
respects to our experiment, perhaps our informed taste testers'
opinions were simply a reflection of the memes they have inherited
from their surroundings... from what they have seen and heard in
advertisements and from those around them. Thus, it is not surprising
that the majority of the informed taste testers, daily bombarded with
memes promoting Coca Cola Classic, almost immediately selected Coca
Cola Classic as their preferred cola... for in a game of "survival of
the fittest," the squeaky wheel most often gets the oil. Though we
like to believe that taste is entirely subjective, it is indeed,
largely manipulated and controlled by society. As Blackmore contends
and our experiment suggests, we do not shape ourselves; memes shape
us.
In conducting our experiment and writing our report, our group made
numerous assumptions about society, the taste testers and the
results. In identifying these assumptions, however, we can account
for the discrepancies that may have occurred in our experiment.
Firstly, we assumed all of our taste testers have the same taste:
that they look for the same qualities in soda (i.e. carbonation
level, sweetness, etc.) or that they even like soda at all. It was
impossible to know what each person tasted and eliminate the
different factors that cause one to choose one soda over the other,
thus making it more difficult to assess society's influence. We
learned in biology, for example, that some people are unable to taste
certain ingredients that produce a sweet or bitter taste. Therefore,
discrepancies in one's preference may be linked to biology and our
genes as opposed to the "memes" of society. We also assumed our taste
testers would not recognize the taste or scent of either soda, and
automatically choose a soda hasQd on this recognition or assumption
rather than their instinctive taste and preference. In addition, due
to our time limitations, we were unable to test a large group of
people. Although the taste testers covered a wide variety of ages and
included both males and females, the results of our experiment may
not accurately reflect what the results of a larger sample size would
be. We furthermore ruled out the possibility of coincidence, assuming
that the people who chose Coca Cola Classic in Test #2 had been
exposed to the media. We assumed that the people who participated in
our experiment had been affected by their exposure to the media, and
thus drew the conclusion that external forces such as "memes" solely
shape our opinion and preferences rather than our true "selves."
No experiment can be complete without identifying the error that
could have factored into the experiment. Therefore, in addition to
recognizing our assumptions, we felt it important to identify the
factors that may have skewed our data, most of which had to do with
the individual sodas. After completing our experiment, for example,
we realized that the expiration date, temperature, dilution, and
carbonation of each soda could have affected a person's preference.
The aforementioned list was not accounted for in most of our
conclusions, and we assumed that those factors were held constant in
each trial. Yet another factor that we must consider is that we
interpreted our data with our hypothesis fresh in our minds.
The implications and consequences of the results of our experiment
are almost overwhelming. Perhaps the most potent implication is that
we are a product of society rather than one of our own creation. This
realization raises the conflict which lies at the heart of
existentialist thought: how do we function in the world with the
knowledge that life is futile? For, we live our lives believing that
we are indeed living our lives... that we are making our own
decisions, thinking our own thoughts, creating our own "selves." This
sense of control over our lives is what keeps many of us going; it is
how we justify and deal with adversity. Yet, how do we function with
the knowledge that we are controlled more by society than by
ourselves? Like Sisyphus in Sartre's "The Myth of Sisyphus" and
Frankl in Man's Search for Meaning we must learn to triumph in
the face of despair by creating our own meaning. We must accept our
lack of control and the absurdity of our lives but we cannot let this
realization hinder us to the point of giving up. We still must
examine our assumptions, see past stereotypes and forge ahead in our
quest for meaning, our search for our "selves."
We designed our experiment to test the cultural assumption that the
more expensive, more popular products are of a higher quality than
cheaper, more generic brands. Before we even began our experiment, we
predicted that we would find proof that this cultural assumption
exists; but that it is indeed only an assumption. Thus, as we
conducted our experiment and analyzed our results, we had our
hypothesis (what we thought would happen) fresh in our minds. After
we performed our experiment, we analyzed our results in terms of our
hypothesis, meaning we checked to see whether our results proved or
disproved our hypothesis. Thus, we analyzed our results from a rather
focused, and thus limited, perspective. Thus, due to our parochial
analysis, it is quite possible that other conclusions could have been
drawn from our results. Once we realized that our results, for the
most part, supported our hypothesis, we turned to the texts we have
studied throughout the semester and tried to draw connections between
what we read and learned, and what the results of our experiment
implied in order to explain and justify our results. It was largely
these connections we were able to make that directed our thinking and
guided our logic, allowing us to thoroughly ans systematically
explore the underlying assumptions, implications, and consequences of
the results of our experiment.