Arons, Arnold B.
Chapter 13
(from A Guide to Introductory Physics Teaching. Wiley. 1990; based on the article "Critical Thinking and the Baccalaureate Curriculum." Association of American Colleges, Liberal Education 71 (1985): 141-157)
(Permission to post on the Punahou CTI Site granted by John
Wiley and Sons.)
The simple but difficult arts of paying attention, copying
accurately, following an argument, detecting an ambiguity or a false
inference, testing guesses by summoning up contrary instances,
organizing one's time and one's thought for study - all these arts .
. . cannot be taught in the air but only through the difficulties of
a defined subject; they cannot be taught in one course in one year.
but must be acquired gradually in dozens of connections.
- Jacques Barzun
13.1 INTRODUCTION
No curricular recommendation, reform, or proposed structure has ever
been made without some obeisance to the generic term "critical
thinking" or one of its synonyms. The flood of reports on education
in our schools and colleges that has been unleashed in recent years
is no exception; every report, at every level of education, calls for
attention to the enhancement of thinking-reasoning capacities in the
young. A currently prominent formula is "higher order thinking
skills." Few of the documents that come to us, however, attempt to
supply some degree of specificity - some operational definition of
the concept, with illustrations of what might be done in day-to-day
teaching to move toward the enunciated goals.
It is the object of this chapter to try to "unpack" the term
"critical thinking" - to list a few simpler, underlying processes of
abstract logical reasoning that are common to many disciplines and
that can be cultivated and exercised separately in limited contexts
accessible to the student. Subsequently, the individual's conscious
weaving together of these various modes results in the larger
synthesis we might characterize as "critical thought." As Barzun
points out in the quotation cited above, this can be done only
through practice in, preferably, more than one field of subject
matter.
13.2 A LIST OF PROCESSES
To glimpse some of the ways in which effective schooling might
enhance students' reasoning capacities, it is instructive to examine
a few of the thinking and reasoning processes that underlie analysis
and inquiry. These are processes that teachers rarely articulate or
point out to students; yet these processes are implicit in many
different studies. The following listing is meant to be illustrative;
it is neither exhaustive nor prescriptive. Readers are invited to add
or elaborate items they have identified for themselves or sense to be
more immediately relevant in their own disciplines.
l Consciously raising the questions "What do we know . . . ? How
do we know . . . ? Why do we accept or believe . . . ? What is the
evidence for . . . ?" when studying some body of material or
approaching a problem.
Consider the assertion, which virtually every student and adult will
make, that the moon shines by reflected sunlight. How many people are
able to describe the simple evidence, available to anyone who can
see, that leads to this conclusion (which was, incidentally,
perfectly clear to the ancients)? This does not require esoteric
intellectual skills; young children can follow and understand; all
one need do is lead them to watch the locations of both the sun and
moon, not just the moon alone, as a few days go by. Yet for the
majority of our population the "fact" that the moon shines by
reflected sunlight is received knowledge, not sustained by
understanding.
Exactly the same must be said about the contention that the earth and
planets revolve around the sun. The validation and acceptance of this
view marked a major turning point in our intellectual history and in
our collective view of man's place in the universe. Although the
basis on which this view is held is more subtle and complex than that
for the illumination of the moon, the "How do we know . . . ?" should
be an intrinsic part of general education; it is, for most people,
however, received knowledge as is also the view that matter is
discrete in its structure rather than continuous.
Similar questions should be asked and addressed in other disciplines:
How does the historian come to know how the Egyptians, or
Babylonians, or Athenians lived? On what basis does the text make
these assertions concerning consequences of the revocation of the
Edict of Nantes? What is the evidence for the claim that such and
such tax and monetary policies promote economic stability? What was
the basis for acceptance of the doctrine of separation of church and
state in our political system?
Cognitive development researchers describe two principal classes of
knowledge: figurative or declarative on the one hand, and operative
or procedural on the other. Declarative knowledge consists of knowing
"facts" (matter is composed of atoms and molecules; animals breathe
oxygen and expel carbon dioxide; the United States entered the Second
World War after the Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor in December
1941). Operative knowledge involves understanding where the
declarative knowledge comes from or what underlies it (What is the
evidence that the structure of matter is discrete rather than
continuous? What do we mean by the terms "oxygen" and "carbon
dioxide" and how do we recognize these as different substances? What
worldwide political and economic events underlay the American
declaration of war?). And operative knowledge also involves the
capacity to use, apply, transform, or recognize the relevance of
declarative knowledge in new situations.
"Above all things," says Alfred North Whitehead in a well-known
passage on the first page of The Aims of Education, "we must beware
of what I will call 'inert ideas' - that is to say, ideas that are
merely received into the mind without being utilized, or tested, or
thrown into fresh combinations." And John Gardner once deplored our
tendency to "to hand our students the cut flowers while forbidding
them to see the growing plants."
Preschool children almost always ask "How do we know . . . ? Why do
we believe . . . ?" questions until formal education teaches them not
to. Most high school and college students then have to be pushed,
pulled, and cajoled into posing and examining such questions; they do
not do so spontaneously. Rather, our usual pace of assignments and
methods of testing all too frequently drive students into memorizing
end results, rendering each development inert. Yet given time and
encouragement, the habit of inquiry can be cultivated, the skill
enhanced, and the satisfaction of understanding conveyed. The effect
would be far more pronounced and development far more rapid if this
demand were made deliberately and simultaneously in science,
humanities, history, and social science courses rather than being
left to occur sporadically, if at all, in one course or
discipline.
2 Being clearly and explicitly aware of gaps in available
information. Recognizing when a conclusion is reached or a decision
made in absence of complete information and being able to tolerate
the attendant ambiguity and uncertainty. Recognizing when one is
taking something on faith without having examined the "How do we know
. . . ? Why do we believe. . . ?" questions.
Interesting investigations of cognitive skill and maturity are
conducted by administering test questions or problems in which some
necessary datum or bit of information has been deliberately omitted,
and the question cannot be answered without securing the added
information or making some plausible assumption that closes the gap.
Most students and many mature adults perform very feebly on these
tests. They have had little practice in such analytical thinking and
fail to recognize, on their own, that information is missing. If they
are told that this is the case, some will identify the gap on
reexamining the problem, but many will still fail to make the
specific identification.
In our subject matter courses, regardless of how carefully we try to
examine evidence and validate our models and concepts, it will
occasionally be necessary to ask students to take something on faith
This is a perfectly reasonable thing to do, but it should never be
done without making students aware of what evidence is lacking and
exactly what they are taking on faith. Without such care, they do not
establish a frame of reference from which to judge their level of
knowledge, and they fail to discriminate clearly those instances in
which evidence has been provided from those in which it has not.
3 Discriminating between observation and inference, between
established fact and subsequent conjecture.
Many students have great trouble making such discriminations even
when the situation seems patently obvious to the teacher. They are
unused to keeping track of the logical sequence, and they are
frequently confused by technical jargon they have previously been
exposed to but never clearly understood.
In the case of the source of illumination of the moon cited earlier,
for example, students must be made explicitly conscious of the fact
that they see the extent of illumination increasing steadily as the
angular separation between moon and sun increases, up to full
illumination at a separation of 180°. This direct observation
leads, in turn, to the inference that what we are seeing is reflected
sunlight.
In working up to the concept of "oxygen" (without any prior mention
of this term at all) with a group of elementary school teachers some
years ago, I had them do an experiment in which they heated red,
metallic copper in an open crucible and weighed the crucible
periodically. What they saw happening, of course, was the copper
turning black and the weight of crucible and contents steadily
increasing. When I walked around the laboratory and asked what they
had observed so far, many answered, "We observed oxygen combining
with the copper." When I quizzically inquired whether that was what
they had actually seen happening, their reaction was one of
puzzlement. It took a sequence of Socratic questioning to lead them
to state what they had actually seen and to discern the inference
that something from the air must be joining the copper to make the
increasing amount of black material in the crucible. It had to be
brought out explicitly that this "something from the air" was the
substance to which we would eventually give the name "oxygen." What
they wanted to do was to use the technical jargon they had acquired
previously without having formed an awareness of what justified
it.
This episode illustrates the importance of exposing students to
repeated opportunity to discriminate between observation and
inference. One remedial encounter in one subject matter context is
not nearly enough, but opportunities are available at almost every
turn. Mendel's observations of nearly integral ratios of population
members having different color and size characteristics must be
separated from inference of the existence of discrete elements
controlling inheritance. In the study of literature, analysis of the
structure of a novel or a poem must be distinguished from an
interpretation of the work. In the study of history, primary
historical data or information cited by the historian must be
separated from the historian's interpretation of the data.
A powerful exercise once employed by some of my colleagues in history
was to give the students a copy of the Code of Hammurabi accompanied
by the assignment: "Write a short paper addressing the following
question: From this code of laws, what can you infer about how these
people lived and what they held to be of value?" This exercise
obviously combines exposure to both processes 1 and 3.
4 Recognizing that words are symbols for ideas and not the ideas
themselves. Recognizing the necessity of using only words of prior
definition, rooted in shared experience, in forming a new definition
and in avoiding being misled by technical jargon.
From the didactic manner in which concepts (particularly scientific
concepts) are forced on students in early schooling, it is little
wonder that they acquire almost no sense of the process of
operational definition and that they come to view concepts as rigid,
unchanging entities with only one absolute significance that the
initiated automatically "know" and that the breathless student must
acquire in one intuitive gulp. It comes as a revelation and a
profound relief to many students when they are allowed to see that
concepts evolve; that they go through a sequence of redefinition,
sharpening, and refinement; that one starts at crude, initial,
intuitive levels and, profiting from insights gained in successive
applications, develops the concept to final sophistication.
In my own courses, I indicate from the first day that we will operate
under the precept "idea first and name afterwards" and that
scientific terms acquire meaning only through the description of
shared experience in words of prior definition. When students try to
exhibit erudition (or take refuge from questioning) by name dropping
technical terms that have not yet been defined, I and my staff go
completely blank and uncomprehending. Students catch on to this game
quite quickly. They cease name dropping and begin to recognize, on
their own, when they do not understand the meaning of a term. Then
they start drifting in to tell us of instances in which they got into
trouble in a psychology, or sociology, or economics, or political
science course by asking for operational meaning of technical terms.
It is interesting that this is an aspect of cognitive development to
which many students break through relatively quickly and easily.
Unfortunately, this is not true of most other modes of abstract
logical reasoning.
5 Probing for assumptions (particularly the implicit,
unarticulated assumptions) behind a line of reasoning.
In science courses, this is relatively easy to do. Idealizations,
approximations, and simplifications lie close to the surface and are
quite clearly articulated in most presentations. They are ignored or
overlooked by the students, however, principally because explicit
recognition and restatement are rarely, if ever, called for on tests
or examinations. In history, humanities, and the social sciences,
underlying assumptions are frequently more subtle and less clearly
articulated; probing for them requires careful and self-conscious
attention on the part of instructors and students.
6 Drawing inferences from data, observations, or other evidence
and recognizing when firm inferences cannot be drawn. This subsumes a
number of processes such as elementary syllogistic reasoning (e.g.,
dealing with basic propositional, "if . . . then" statements),
correlational reasoning, recognizing when relevant variables have or
have not been controlled.
Separate from the analysis of another's line of reasoning is the
formulation of one's own. "If . . . then" reasoning from data or
information must be undertaken without prompting from an external
"authority." One must be able to discern possible cause-and-effect
relations in the face of statistical scatter and uncertainty. One
must be aware that failure to control a significant variable vitiates
the possibility of inferring a cause-and-effect relation. One must be
able to discern when two alternative models, explanations, or
interpretations are equally valid and cannot be discriminated on
logical grounds alone.
As an illustration of the latter situation, I present a case I
encounter very frequently in my own teaching. When students in a
general education science course begin to respond to assignments
leading them to watch events in the sky (diurnal changes in rising,
setting, and elevation of the sun, waxing and waning of the moon,
behavior of the stars and readily visible planets), they immediately
expect these naked eye observations to allow them to "see" the
"truth" they have received from authority, namely that the earth and
planets revolve around the sun. When they first confront the fact
that both the geo- and heliocentric models rationalize the
observations equally well and that it is impossible to eliminate one
in favor of the other on logical grounds at this level of
observation, they are quite incredulous. They are shocked by the
realization that either model might be selected provisionally on the
basis of convenience, or of aesthetic or religious predilection. In
their past experience, there has always been a pat answer. They have
never been led to stand back and recognize that one must sometimes
defer, either temporarily or permanently, to unresolvable
alternatives. They have never had to wait patiently until sufficient
information and evidence were accumulated to develop an answer to an
important question; the answer has always been asserted (for the sake
of "closure") whether the evidence was at hand or not, and the
ability to discriminate decidability versus undecidability has never
evolved.
An essentially parallel situation arises in the early stages of
formation of the concepts of static electricity (see Sections 6.7 and
6.8). Students are very reluctant to accept the fact that, before we
know anything about the microscopic constitution of matter and the
role of electrical charge at that level, it is impossible to tell
from observable (macroscopic) phenomena whether positive charge,
negative charge, or both charges are mobile or being displaced. They
wish to be told the "right answer" and fail to comprehend that any
one of the three models accounts equally well for what we have
observed and predicts equally well in new situations. They want to
use the term "electron" even though they have no idea what it means
or what evidence justifies it, and they apply it incorrectly to
irrelevant and inappropriate situations.
If attention is explicitly given, experiences such as the ones just
outlined can play a powerful role in opening student minds to
spontaneous assessment of what they know and what they do not know,
of what can be inferred at a given juncture and what cannot.
7 Performing hypothetico-deductive reasoning; that is, given a
particular situation, applying relevant knowledge of principles and
constraints and visualizing, in the abstract, the plausible outcomes
that might result from various changes one can imagine to be imposed
on the system.
Opportunities for such thinking abound in almost every course. Yet
students are most frequently given very circumscribed questions that
do not open the door to more imaginative hypothetico-deductive
reasoning. The restricted situations are important and provide
necessary exercises as starting points, but they should be followed
by questions that impel the student to invent possible changes and
pursue the plausible consequences.
8 Discriminating between inductive and deductive reasoning; that
is, being aware when an argument is being made from the particular to
the general or from the general to the particular.
The concepts of "electric circuit," "electric current," and
"resistance" can be induced from very simple observations made with
electric batteries and arrangements of flashlight bulbs. This leads
to the inductive construction of a "model" of operation of an
electric circuit. The model then forms the basis for deductive
reasoning, that is, predictions of what will happen to brightness of
bulbs in new configurations or when changes (such as short
circuiting) are imposed on an existing configuration.
Exactly similar thinking can be developed in connection with economic
models or processes. Hypothetico-deductive reasoning is intimately
involved in virtually all such instances, but one should always be
fully conscious of the distinction between the inductive and the
deductive modes.
9 Testing one's own line of reasoning and conclusions for internal
consistency and thus developing intellectual self-reliance.
The time is long past when we could teach our students all they need
to know. The principal function of education - higher education in
particular - must be to help individuals to their own intellectual
feet: To give them conceptual starting points and an awareness of
what it means to learn and understand something so that they can
continue to read, study, and learn as need and opportunity arise,
without perpetual formal instruction.
To continue genuine learning on one's own (not just accumulating
facts) requires the capacity to judge when understanding has been
achieved and to draw conclusions and make inferences from acquired
knowledge. Inferring, in turn, entails testing one's own thinking,
and the results of such thinking, for correctness or at least for
internal coherence and consistency. This is, of course, a very
sophisticated level of intellectual activity, and students must first
be made aware of the process and its importance. Then they need
practice and help.
In science courses, they should be required to test and verify
results and conclusions by checking that the results make sense in
extreme or special cases that can be reasoned out simply and
directly. They should be led to solve a problem in alternative ways
when that is possible. Such thinking should be conducted in both
quantitative and qualitative situations. In the humanities and social
sciences, the checks for internal consistency are more subtle, but
they are equally important and should be cultivated explicitly.
Students should be helped to sense when they can be confident of the
soundness, consistency, or plausibility of their own reasoning so
that they can consciously dispense with the teacher and cease relying
on someone else for the "right answer."
10 Developing self-consciousness concerning one's own thinking and
reasoning processes.
This is perhaps the highest and most sophisticated reasoning skill,
presupposing the others that have been listed. It involves standing
back and recognizing the processes one is using, deliberately
invoking those most appropriate to the given circumstances, and
providing the basis for conscious transfer of reasoning methods from
familiar to unfamiliar contexts.
Given such awareness, one can begin to penetrate new situations by
asking oneself probing questions and constructing answers. Starting
with artificial, idealized, oversimplified versions of the problem,
one can gradually penetrate to more realistic and complex versions.
In an important sense, this is the mechanism underlying independent
research and investigation.
13.3 WHY BOTHER WITH CRITICAL THINKING?
The preceding list of thinking and reasoning processes underlying the
broad generic term "critical thinking" is neither complete nor
exhaustive. For illustrative purposes, I have tried to isolate and
describe processes and levels of awareness that appear to be bound up
with clear thinking and genuine understanding in a wide variety of
disciplines and to show a deep commonality in this respect among very
different kinds of subject matter. These processes underlie the
capacity defined by Jacques Barzun in the quotation that heads this
chapter.
Developing these intellectual skills requires extensive, sustained
practice. Such practice is not possible in a space devoid of subject
matter. It is only through contact with, and immersion in, rich areas
of subject matter that interesting and significant experience can be
generated. Although it may be possible, in principle, to generate
limited aspects of such practice through artificial kinds of
exercises and puzzle solving, or even through analysis of scores in
sports contests, it seems a waste of time to resort to such sterile
channels when all the vital disciplines of our culture lie at our
disposal.
Why should we want to cultivate skills such as those I have listed?
There are many obvious reasons having to do with quality of life,
with professional competence, with the advance of culture and of
society in general, but I particularly wish to suggest a
socio-political reason: the education of an enlightened democratic
citizenry. What capacities characterize such a citizenry?
Justice Learned Hand, the distinguished jurist of the preceding
generation, argued with telling irony that we would be able to
preserve civil liberties only so long as we were willing to engage in
the "intolerable labor thought, that most distasteful of all our
activities." John Dewey in Democracy and Education contends that "The
opposite to thoughtful action are routine or capricious behavior.
Both refuse to acknowledge responsibility for the future consequences
which flow from present action."
The requirements set by Barzun, Hand, and Dewey can be broken down to
more fundamental components. The sophisticated distinction between
enlightened and short range self-interest is based on
hypothetico-deductive reasoning. Such reasoning is also inevitably
involved in visualizing possible outcomes of decisions and policies
in economic and political domains.
There is need to discriminate between facts and inferences in the
contentions with which one is surrounded. There is the necessity of
making tentative judgments or decisions, and it is better that this
be done in full awareness of gaps in available information than in an
illusion of certainty. There is the highly desirable capacity to ask
critical, probing, fruitful questions concerning situations in which
one has little or no expertise. There is the need to be explicitly
conscious of the limits of one's own knowledge and understanding on a
given issue.
Each of these capacities appears on the preceding list, and I believe
that each can be cultivated and enhanced, at least to some degree, in
the great majority of college students through properly designed
experiences embracing a wide variety of subjects.
I hasten to emphasize that these skills alone are not sufficient to
assure good citizenship or other desirable qualities of mind and
person. Other ingredients are necessary, not the least of which are
moral and ethical values, which impose their own constraints on the
naked processes of thinking and reasoning. Although values are not
disconnected from thinking and reasoning, the educational problems
they pose transcend the limits of this short essay and require
discussion in their own right.
13.4 EXISTING LEVEL OF CAPACITY FOR ABSTRACT LOGICAL
REASONING
In the United States some investigators have rather belatedly come to
realize that much of our science curricular material, and the volume
and pace with which we thrust it at our students, are badly
mismatched to the existing levels of student intellectual development
at virtually every age. I am convinced that the same is true in other
disciplines, but the fact is less readily discerned because
assignments and tests concentrate on end results and procedures
rather than on reasoning and understanding.
I say that "some" have become aware of this problem because, despite
the unequivocal and relentlessly accumulating statistics, many who
teach in the schools, colleges, and universities remain unaware of
the emerging data; others fail to see any relevance to their own
teaching.
Beginning about 1971, investigators began administering elementary
tasks in abstract logical reasoning (such as those pioneered by Jean
Piaget in his studies of the development of abstract reasoning
capacity in children) to adolescents and adults of college age and
beyond. The tests have centered principally on arithmetical reasoning
with ratios or division and on awareness of the necessity of
controlling variables in deducing cause-effect relationship.
Although the results vary significantly from one population to
another (economically disadvantaged versus economically advantaged;
concentrating in science and engineering versus concentrating in
humanities or fine arts versus concentrating in the social sciences,
etc.), the overall averages have remained essentially unchanged with
increasing volume of data since the first small samples were reported
in 1971, and, most suggestively, the averages do not change
appreciably with increasing age beyond about 12 or 13: Roughly one
third of the total number of individuals tested solve the tasks
correctly; roughly one third perform incorrectly but show a partial,
incipient grasp of the necessary mode of reasoning; the remaining
third fail completely. In Piagetian terminology, the first group
might be described as using formal patterns of reasoning, the third
group as using principally concrete patterns, and the middle group as
being in transition between the two modes.
The weaknesses revealed by these two specific tasks would mean
relatively little if they stood by themselves, but, in fact, these
weaknesses are closely correlated with weaknesses in other modes of
abstract logical reasoning such as discriminating between observation
and inference; dealing with elementary syllogisms involving
inclusion, exclusion, and serial ordering; recognizing gaps in
available information; doing almost any kind of hypothetico-deductive
reasoning.
Most of the curricular materials thrust at students in the majority
of their courses at secondary and college level implicitly require
well-developed reasoning capacity in the modes that have been listed
in this discussion. In fact, only a small proportion of the students
(less than one third) are ready for such performance. The rest,
lacking the steady, supportive help and explicit exercises required,
resort, in desperation, to memorization of end results and
procedures. Failing to develop the processes underlying critical
thinking, they fail to have experience of genuine understanding and
come to believe that knowledge is inculcated by teachers and consists
of recognizing juxtapositions of arcane vocabulary on multiple choice
tests. (Readers familiar with the studies of William G. Perry will
recognize his first category of intellectual outlook among college
students.)
13.5 CAN CAPACITY FOR ABSTRACT LOGICAL REASONING BE
ENHANCED?
In our Physics Education Group at the University of Washington, we
have worked intensively for some years with populations of pre- and
in-service elementary school teachers and other nonscience majors
ranging in age from 18 to over 30. Initially no more than about 10%
were using formal patterns of reasoning. By starting with very basic,
concrete observations and experiences, forming concepts out of such
direct experience, going slowly, allowing students to make and
rectify mistakes by confronting contradiction or inconsistency,
insisting that they speak and write out their lines of reasoning and
explanation, repeating the same modes of reasoning in new contexts
days and weeks apart, we have been able to increase the fraction who
successfully use abstract patterns of reasoning to perhaps 70 to 90%,
depending on the nature of the task.
The most important practical lesson we have learned is that
repetition is absolutely essential - not treading water in the same
context until "mastery" is attained, but in altered and increasingly
richer context, with encounters spread out over time. Quick, remedial
exercises in artificial situations preceding "real" course work are
virtually useless. One must patiently construct repeated encounters
with the same modes of reasoning in regular course work and allow
students to benefit from their mistakes. Progress becomes clearly
visible in the sense that the percentage of successful students
increases with each repetition.
It is still a very long step from the development of specific
abstract reasoning processes in one area of subject matter, such as
elementary science, to more advanced levels of subject matter in the
same area, not to speak of transfer to entirely different areas. What
little evidence exists suggests that very little transfer occurs from
experience acquired in only one discipline. I myself am strongly
convinced, however (mostly by fragmentary, anecdotal evidence, and
perhaps some admixture of wishful thinking), that very great progress
could be effected if students were simultaneously exposed to such
intellectual experience in entirely different disciplines. This is
largely a matter of conjecture since an organized experiment at the
college level has not really been tried.
The fragmentary evidence to which I appeal comes from two disparate
sources:
1 Experience with a tightly organized core curriculum at Amherst
College during the '50s and '60s: In this curriculum, there was a
very strong interaction among an English composition course, a
science course, an American Studies course, and, toward the later
stages, a Western Civilization course, all of which had certain
attitudes, approaches, and intellectual standards in common. Alumni
of that period tend to comment very favorably, in retrospect, on the
effect of that experience on the own intellectual development. (So
tightly organized a curriculum was a rather special case and, as then
implemented, would be possible only with a small, homogeneous student
body. Judicious modifications should, however, be effective in more
heterogeneous situations.)
2 Data being reported on effects of the elementary school science
curricula developed under auspices of the National Science Foundation
during the '60s. The latter evidence is very indirect, but it is
highly suggestive and merits a bit of discussion. The groups that
developed the new curricula worked directly with the children they
sought to teach and met the latter on their own ground and at their
existing verbal and conceptual starting points rather than in some
never-never land of unchecked and untested hypotheses and assumptions
about children and learning. Everything in these materials begins
with hands-on experience and observation. Concepts are developed
through induction and synthesis from this experience, with the
teacher as guide and pilot rather than as verbal inculcator. Ideas
are developed first and names are invented afterwards; technical
terms are generated operationally only after experience has given
them sanction and meaning.
The essence of instruction in these programs, whether the subject
matter is physical or biological, is to give the children time time
to explore, to test, to manipulate, to talk and argue about meaning
and interpretation, to articulate hypotheses, to follow trails to
dead ends and retrace steps if necessary, to make mistakes and to
revise views and interpretations when guided to perceive
contradictions (instead of being told, by assertion, that their idea
was "right" or "wrong"), to decide when and how arithmetical
calculations should be made.
Such learning is sometimes (misleadingly) called "discovery
learning." The children are, of course, not expected to be Newtons,
Faradays, Agassizes, or Darwins, "discovering" the concepts and
theories of science de novo by the age of ten. Ordinary, lively,
curious children simply react positively to the opportunity to learn
from perceptively guided experience and observation. They retain what
they learn because they are synthesizing genuine experience rather
than memorizing a jumble of meaningless and unfamiliar words. They
know where their knowledge comes from and are able to address the
"How do we know . . . ? Why do we believe . . . ?" questions.
Since the first appearance of these curricula, researchers have been
comparing the achievement of children exposed to such materials with
the achievement of controls. In addition to showing significantly
improved command of science subject matter, children exposed to the
new- curricula show significantly greater progress in both verbal and
numerical skills, and the effects are particularly strong among
disadvantaged children. In other words, this mode of instruction,
when competently implemented, results in transfer, enhancing
performance beyond the science subject matter alone.
Although there is no direct evidence of a similar kind supporting the
notion that we would enhance the higher level reasoning skills of
college students by undertaking the instructional effort I have been
advocating, I submit that the observations of the effect of the
inquiry-oriented science curricula on children are at least very
encouraging. The processes involved are analogous, and the effort
seems worth making.
13.6 CONSEQUENCES OF MISMATCH
We are indeed fortunate that a significant proportion of our student
population, perhaps one quarter, does make the breakthrough on modes
of abstract logical reasoning spontaneously. (Consider the
consequences to our society if this were otherwise!) But this does
not lessen the urgency of improving our performance. As pointed out
earlier, there now exists a serious mismatch between curricular
materials and expectations on the one hand and actual level of
student intellectual development on the other. The curricular
materials implicitly require abstract reasoning capacities and levels
of insight and interpretation that many students have not yet
attained. Neither the materials nor the most prevalent modes of
instruction provide the gently paced insistent, repetitive guidance
that is necessary for helping students develop the necessary
intellectual skills.
This mismatch has extensive deleterious consequences. We force a
large fraction of students into blind memorization by imposing on
them, particularly at high school and university levels, materials
requiring abstract reasoning capacities they have not yet attained.
And we proceed through these materials at a pace that precludes
effective learning and understanding, even if the necessary reasoning
capacities have been formed. Under such pressure, students acquire no
experience of what understanding really entails. They cannot test
their "knowledge" for plausible consequences or for internal
consistency; they have no sense of where accepted ideas or results
come from, how they are validated, or why they are to be accepted or
believed. In other words, they do not have the opportunity to develop
the habits of critical thinking defined earlier in this essay, and
they acquire the misapprehension that knowledge resulted in memorized
assertions, esoteric technical terminology, and regurgitation of
received "facts." Although such failure is widely prevalent in the
sciences, it is by no means confined there. It pervades our entire
system, including history, the humanities, and the social
sciences.
One specific example of the mechanism through which an entire system
becomes degraded emerges through our experience with arithmetical
reasoning. When I first discovered that no more than 10% of my
undergraduate nonscience majors could reason arithmetically with
division, I wondered what had happened to the old word problems that
were used to cultivate such reasoning from the fifth and sixth grades
on. Going back to existing elementary school arithmetic texts, I
found that such problems were still there, as in my own school days,
and were probably significantly improved. When I questioned my
university students, they began to reveal that they had never
actually had to do such problems in school because the problems were
"too hard." When I began working with in-service elementary school
teachers and found that they themselves could not deal with such
problems, the pattern was clear: An engineer would describe the
system as a "degenerative feedback loop." The arithmetical reasoning
disability of the future teachers had never been detected and
remedied when they were at the university. They graduated, went into
the schools, and passed their disability and fear to most of the
children by not requiring the doing of the word problems and
conveying the rationalization that they were "too hard." The children
went on to the university, and so on and so forth.
The case of arithmetical reasoning is just an especially clear and
vivid illustration. The same pattern arises over and over again in
other instances: In failure to master and understand the most
fundamental scientific concepts (such as velocity and acceleration or
the nature of floating and sinking); in poor writing and speaking of
English; in incapacity to deal with historical reasoning and the
concomitant blind concentration of historical "facts."
I wish to emphasize most strongly that the teachers whose
incapacities I describe are not the ones to be blamed for this
situation. The input terminals to the feedback loop of my metaphor
reside in our hands at the colleges and universities. We are the ones
who perpetuate the mismatch and fail to provide remediation of
disabilities and enhancement of abstract reasoning capacities at the
opportunities that we control. We are the ones who made the teachers
as they are.
The mismatch about which I complain affects, of course, not only our
future teachers but the whole of our student population outside the
25% who, in spite of the system, manage to break through
spontaneously to abstract reasoning patterns. I dwell so insistently
on the teachers only because of the crucial role they play in
sustaining the feedback loop. Think of the prodigious impetus that
might stem from altering the condition of the teachers and making the
feedback regenerative instead of degenerative! What might we be able
to achieve at university level if the mismatch between our materials
and student readiness were removed?
13.7 ASCERTAINING STUDENT DIFFICULTIES
It might be helpful to point out some hard facts regarding the
securing of reliable information concerning student learning
difficulties and levels of abstract reasoning. What one must learn to
do is ask simple, sequential questions, leading students in a
deliberate Socratic fashion. After each question, one must shut up
and listen carefully to the response. (It is the tendency of most
inexperienced questioners to provide an answer, or to change the
question, if a response is not forthcoming within one second. One
must learn to wait as long as four or five seconds, and one then
finds that students, having been given a chance to think, will
respond in sentences and truly reveal their lines of thought.)
As the students respond to such careful questioning, one can begin to
discern the errors, misconceptions, and missteps in logic that are
prevalent. One learns nothing by giving students "right answers" or
"lucid explanations." As a matter of fact, students do not benefit
from such answers or explanations; they simply memorize them.
Students are much more significantly helped when they are led to
confront contradictions and inconsistencies in what they say and then
spontaneously alter their own statements as a result of such
confrontation.
In such dialogs, two things immediately strike novice investigators.
First they find that virtually all of their a priori conjectures
concerning what students are and are not thinking are incorrect and
that entirely unanticipated but very fundamental, plausible, and
deeply rooted preconceptions, misconceptions, and misapprehensions
(of which the investigator had no awareness) are revealed. Second,
they discover the saving grace in all of this unanticipated
complexity: The frequently voiced cliché that every individual
is completely different from every other individual is patently
untrue. Each kind of misconception or erroneous mode of reasoning
occurs, with remarkable reproducibility, in many individuals. Some
hurdles and misconceptions are very widely prevalent. When one finds
an approach or insight that overcomes a particular difficulty, that
approach will be helpful not to only one but to many individuals.
It must be strongly emphasized that conclusions must be based on
careful and accurate listening to students. Casual extrapolation of
one's own experience only leads to error. Those of us who are
fortunate enough to have become competent professionals are among the
25% minority mentioned earlier. We made the breakthrough in spite of
the system, not because of it. Our own learning experiences are not
representative, and citing such experience rarely leads to correct
insight into what transpires for the majority of learners.
13.8 TESTING
At present, deficiency in the quality of testing is one of the more
serious ills of our profession. There is a large and perceptive
literature on testing in virtually every discipline, but its
influence has, unfortunately, not been extensive. Some cynics have
even remarked on the existence of a destructive collusion between
students and teachers - a collusion in which students agree to accept
bad teaching provided they are given bad examinations.
It is useless to render lip service to sophisticated intellectual
goals and then test only for end results, vocabulary, "facts," or
"information." The real goals of a course are determined not by what
we say but what we test for. Students quickly ascertain what the real
requirements of a course are and orient their efforts accordingly.
Their attention can be focussed on the higher intellectual processes
and requirements only if these aspects are included in testing and
writing and play an important role in the final grade.
It is my earnest hope that more self-conscious attention to thinking
and reasoning processes on the part of faculty will lead to
statistical improvement in the quality of test questions and writing
assignments. Good questions are very hard to devise, and any one
individual runs out of inspiration. Collaborative effort could
greatly increase the pool of good material and also provide the
debugging that is always necessary.
13.9 SOME THOUGHTS ON FACULTY DEVELOPMENT
Given the almost universally accepted goal of enhancing the capacity
for critical thinking in our students, it seems reasonable to lead
faculty members to sharpen their own critical thinking about how this
goal is to be attained through the use of units of subject matter in
their own areas of expertise units which they have taught and with
which they feel comfortable. The problem is to get away from vague,
mushy generalizations and to provide constraints that induce
consideration and elaboration of very specific examples: analyzing a
unit of subject matter so as to identify the thinking and reasoning
processes that must be brought to bear by the student, and devising
questions that lead the student to such penetration.
I suggest that useful results might stem from the organization of
faculty workshops in which participants, working in pairs, come
prepared with a response to something like the following
assignment:
(a) Select from within your area of expertise a unit of subject matter (or a laboratory experiment) with which you are thoroughly familiar and study of which, you believe, will help a student attain a particular intellectual goal or insight or that will serve to exercise a particular reasoning process. (The unit should be as short as possible, but it should have a significant goal and not end up as a triviality.)
(b) State the goals or insights that you discern.
(c) Describe the essence of the unit of subject matter; that is, indicate how it provides a path toward the goal or insight.
(d) List the various abstract reasoning capacities that the student must already possess, or must be helped to develop, in order to deal properly with the subject matter and not have to resort to memorization. (The basis for this analysis might be the list provided earlier in this chapter or an appropriately modified or augmented list.)
(e) Indicate the kind of help you might provide students who encounter difficulty penetrating the material (e.g., questions that help point up significant issues, or clarify concepts, or focus on assumptions that are likely to be overlooked).
(f) Indicate what writing you might ask the students to do in connection with the unit and how you would test for final mastery or understanding.
(g) Indicate how you would lead the students to stand back, become conscious of the patterns of thinking and reasoning in which they had engaged, and, if possible, connect this experience with experiences they have had in other courses.
I imagine the workshop as bringing together pairs of individuals
from the same and different disciplines. Each pair would present
their analysis for discussion and comment by the entire group. There
need be no "expert" or "authority" directing the proceedings. I would
like to think that faculty members seriously interested in the
intellectual development of their students would find such an
exercise interesting and stimulating. They would become more
conscious of commonalities across disciplinary lines while defining
real differences more precisely. The necessity of presenting the
essence of a specific intellectual exercise to colleagues in other
disciplines would help minimize the use of jargon and would sharpen
awareness of how units of subject matter can be utilized. And
finally, the whole enterprise would help cultivate an instructional
climate in which students clearly perceived that they were being
helped to develop their own capacities for critical thinking.